ARCHIVAL REPORT Methylphenidate But Not Atomoxetine or Citalopram Modulates Inhibitory Control and Response Time Variability L. Sanjay Nandam, Robert Hester, Joe Wagner, Tarrant D.R. Cummins, Kelly Garner, Angela J. Dean, Bung Nyun Kim, Pradeep J. Nathan, Jason B. Mattingley, and Mark A. Bellgrove Background: Response inhibition is a prototypical executive function of considerable clinical relevance to psychiatry. Nevertheless, our understanding of its pharmacological modulation remains incomplete. Methods: We used a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover design to examine the effect of an acute dose of methyl- phenidate (MPH) (30 mg), atomoxetine (ATM) (60 mg), citalopram (CIT) (30 mg), and placebo (PLAC) (dextrose) on the stop signal inhibition task in 24 healthy, right-handed men 18 –35 years of age. Participants performed the task under each of the four drug conditions across four consecutive sessions. Results: Methylphenidate led to a reduction in both response time variability and stop-signal reaction time (SSRT), indicating enhanced response inhibition compared with all other drug conditions. Crucially, the enhancement of response inhibition by MPH occurred without concomitant changes in overall response speed, arguing against a simple enhancement of processing speed. We found no significant differences between ATM and PLAC, CIT and PLAC, or ATM and CIT for either response time variability or SSRT. Conclusions: An acute dose of MPH but not ATM or CIT was able to improve SSRT and reduce response time variability in nonclinical participants. Improvements in response inhibition and response variability might underlie the reported clinical benefits of MPH in disorders such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Key Words: Atomoxetine, citalopram, methylphenidate, response
are respected, an index of the speed of inhibition can be calculated,
Reaction time tasks, including the stop-signal task, also allow
Theprocessesthatinhibitunwantedbehaviorandmaintain measurementofbehavioralvariability,measuredastheSDofreac-
consistent task performance are impaired in several psychiat-
tion times to the go signal. Increased variability is thought to arise
ric conditions, including schizophrenia obsessive compul-
from both moment-to-moment fluctuations in attentional control
sive disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
and from more gradual drifts in performance that might result from
(ADHD) Significant controversy exists regarding their precise
neurochemical basis, despite the relevance of response inhibition
Here we sought to determine the influence of three agents that
and variability to psychiatry and their potential for pharmacological
are used in the management of ADHD (MPH, ATM) and obsessive
treatment. Here we used a within-subjects design to examine the
compulsive disorder (CIT) on behavioral measures of response inhi-
influence of an acute dose of methylphenidate (MPH), atomoxetine
bition. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover
(ATM), citalopram (CIT), or PLAC on measures of inhibitory control
design was used to study the effects of an acute dose of MPH (30
mg), ATM (60 mg), CIT (30 mg), and PLAC (dextrose) on SSRT and
Response inhibition has been studied in cognitive neuroscience
behavioral variability in healthy subjects.
with paradigms such as the stop-signal task. This task requires the
Methods and Materials
countermanding or cancelation of a prepotent “go” response uponpresentation of an infrequent “stop” signal. Stop-signal inhibition
Participants
can be viewed as a race between two competing “go” and “stop”
Twenty-four healthy right-handed, nonclinical Caucasian male
processes. By introducing a delay between the presentation of the
participants, 18 –35 years of age, were recruited. Additional details
go and any subsequent stop signal, one can bias the outcome of the
regarding the recruitment and screening procedures can be found
race. When the theoretical assumptions underlying this race model
Drug Administration
From the University of Queensland (LSN, JW, TDRC, KG, AJD, BNK, JBM, MAB),
Participants were tested on the same day and time across 4
Queensland Brain Institute and School of Psychology; Prince CharlesHospital (LSN), Brisbane; University of Melbourne (RH), Department of
consecutive weeks in a double-blind manner. On each occasion a
Psychology, Parkville; Monash University (PJN), School of Psychology,
single blue gelatine capsule containing MPH 30 mg, ATM 60 mg, CIT
Psychiatry and Psychological Medicine, Clayton, Australia; Seoul Na-
30 mg, or PLAC (dextrose) was ingested with water. Participants
tional University (BNK), Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
performed the stop-signal reaction time task from min ϩ150 to
Department of Neuropsychiatry, College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea; and
ϩ180 after drug administration. There was no significant drug ϫ
The University of Cambridge (PJN), Department of Psychiatry, Cam-
time interactions for either blood pressure or subjective side effect
Address correspondence to Mark Bellgrove, Ph.D., Queensland Brain Instit-
ute, The University of Queensland, 4072 Brisbane, Australia. E-mail:
Stop-Signal Paradigm
The SSRT was derived as the mean reaction time to go-stimuli
Received Jun 30, 2010; revised Nov 8, 2010; accepted Nov 8, 2010.
(mean reaction time [MRT]) minus the stop signal delay for the 50%
Table 1. Mean SSRT and MRT and Stop-Signal Accuracy as a Function of Drug Condition
Methylphenidate 30 mg (MPH); atomoxetine 60 mg (ATM); citalopram 30 mg (CIT); placebo (PLAC). SSRT, stop-
signal reaction time (msec); MRT, mean correct “go” reaction time (msec); % stop, percentage of successful inhibitionson stop trials; ICV, intra-individual coefficient of variation (SD of MRT/MRT).
inhibition threshold (SSRT ϭ MRT Ϫ stop-signal delay) The
Methylphenidate is a widely used stimulant medication for the
intraindividual coefficient of variation [ICV: SD Go RT/MRT] was also
treatment of ADHD, with clinical response rates of approximately
calculated, which provides a measure of response variability, ad-
70%. Although MPH is often viewed as a dopaminergic agent, its
justed for the influence of response speed
pharmacology suggests effects on both dopamine (DA) and nor-adrenaline (NA). Within the striatum, MPH acts to inhibit the re-
uptake of DA by blockade of the dopamine transporter (DAT) The increase in DA occasioned by DAT blockade likely modulates
There was a significant effect of drug condition on SSRT
activity within the circuits of the basal ganglia, particularly via D2
[F (3,66) ϭ 5.83, p Ͻ .01]. Methylphenidate led to a reduction in
and D1 receptors within the indirect and direct pathways, respec-
SSRTs, indicating enhanced response inhibition compared with all
tively. However, MPH increases both DA and NA at doses that en-
other drug conditions. Post hoc least significant difference testsrevealed significant differences between MPH and PLAC (p Ͻ .001;
hance prefrontally dependent executive functions such as working
d ϭ .65) and MPH and CIT (p Ͻ .01; d ϭ .62) and between MPH and
memory This effect is likely mediated via blockade of the
ATM (p ϭ .05, d ϭ .42) There were no significant differ-
noradrenaline transporter (NET), because DAT is sparse in prefron-
ences between ATM and PLAC (p ϭ .1; d ϭ .32) or between ATM and
tal cortex At the receptor level, the cognitive enhancing effects
CIT (p ϭ .2; d ϭ .26).
of MPH in rat prefrontal cortex seem to be mediated by its effects on
Crucially, the selective enhancement of response inhibition by
MPH occurred without concomitant changes in response speed,
Pharmacological work in rodents has shown that, although ATM
because there was no main effect of drug condition on MRT
selectively inhibits NET in prefrontal cortex, there is a resultant
[F (3,66) ϭ .54, p ϭ .65]. This suggests that MPH was able to specifi-
threefold increase in both NA and DA levels, without any concomi-
cally improve action cancelation without simply increasing overall
tant change in serotonin However, within the striatum NET is
motor speed. A conservative reanalysis of the SSRT data with MRT as
sparse, and ATM has only a limited ability to modulate catechol-
a covariate confirmed the significant effect of drug condition on
amine levels. Although ATM and MPH have similar effects on both
SSRT [p ϭ .002]. There was no main effect of drug condition on
DA and NA in prefrontal cortex, a key difference is conferred by the
stop-signal accuracy [F (3,66) ϭ 1.35, p ϭ .27]
ability of MPH to selectively increase DA within the striatum
Significant main effects of drug condition were found for the
Current models of behavioral inhibition emphasize the interac-
intraindividual coefficient of variation [F (3,66) ϭ 5.76, p ϭ .001].
tion of prefrontal and basal ganglia circuits Specifically,
Methylphenidate led to a reduction in response time variability that
prefrontal circuits might provide a top-down, stimulus-driven input
was significantly different from all other conditions (all p values Ͻ
to the basal ganglia, signaling the need for enhanced behavioral
.05, corrected) No other drug comparisons of variability
control. Both MPH and ATM are well-placed to exert a neuromodu-
latory influence over the prefrontal cortex, and indeed functional
Because MPH enhanced both SSRT and response time variabil-
magnetic resonance imaging studies of response inhibition dem-
ity, we sought to understand the relationship between these vari-
onstrate effects of both drugs on prefrontal activity Dopa-
ables with correlation. No significant correlations were found be-
mine, however, might play an important neuromodulatory role
tween SSRT and response time variability in any of the drug
within the basal ganglia, acting to transform the top-down inputs
conditions, suggesting that these processes are largely indepen-
into a focused, context-dependent signal that is able to suppress or
dent (MPH: r ϭ .08, p Ͼ .05; ATM: r ϭ .02, p Ͼ .05; CIT: r ϭ .27, p Ͼ .05;
facilitate behavior via the appropriate balance of activity within the
PLAC: r ϭ .32, p Ͼ .05).
indirect or direct pathways, respectively Future studies shouldattempt to modulate SSRT with selective D1/D2 agonists or antag-
Discussion
onists as well as with a broader range of cognitive tasks to accu-
This study demonstrated that clinically relevant doses of MPH
rately reflect the complexity and breadth of the construct of inhibi-
were able to reduce SSRT and behavioral variability without con-
comitant changes in response speed or accuracy of responding.
Recent evidence from ADHD suggests that response time vari-
The inability of CIT to facilitate action cancelation is consistent with
ability and inhibition load onto distinct, familial cognitive factors
other human studies However, the failure to confirm a beneficial
with the former potentially linked to diminished arousal and
effect of ATM compared with PLAC on SSRT contrasts with other
drifting attention and the latter linked to executive processes.
work in humans and rodents Our results challenge the view
Interestingly, although both stop-signal reaction time and re-
that stimulant medications act to solely speed motoric processes
sponse time variability were robustly improved by MPH in the cur-
without specific effects on action cancelation
rent study, these measures were largely uncorrelated in each of the
drug conditions, providing further evidence that they are poten-
6. Johnson KA, Kelly SP, Bellgrove MA, Barry E, Cox M, Gill M, Robertson IH
(2007): Response variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:
Chamberlain et al. reported that an acute dose of ATM 40 mg
Evidence for neuropsychological heterogeneity. Neuropsychologia 45:
reduced SSRT compared with PLAC. A comparison of the effect size
associated with the ATM versus PLAC difference in Chamberlain et
7. Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Blackwell AD, Clark L, Robbins TW, Sahakian
BJ (2006): Neurochemical modulation of response inhibition and prob-
al. and the current study revealed Cohen’s d effect sizes of .37 and
abilistic learning in humans. Science 311:861– 863.
.32, respectively. Because these effect sizes are modest, non-repli-
8. Bari A, Eagle DM, Mar AC, Robinson ES, Robbins TW (2009): Dissociable
cations are likely. It is also notable that the PLAC condition of the
effects of noradrenaline, dopamine, and serotonin uptake blockade on
current study yielded comparable results (Chamberlain: SSRT: 278
stop task performance in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 205:273–283.
msec; Nandam et al.: SSRT: 275 msec), suggesting that baseline
9. Chamberlain SR, Hampshire A, Muller U, Rubia K, Campo ND, Craig K, et
differences between the studies are unlikely to account for this
al. (2008): Atomoxetine modulates right inferior frontal activation dur-
non-replication. Participant factors such as DNA variation in the NET
ing inhibitory control: A pharmacological functional magnetic reso-
genes might also account for the weaker effect of ATM in the
nance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 65:550 –555.
10. Eagle DM, Bari A, Robbins TW (2008): The neuropsychopharmacology of
action inhibition: Cross-species translation of the stop-signal and go/
Although we found strong evidence that MPH specifically im-
no-go tasks. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 199:439 – 456.
proved response inhibition and response time variability, we found
11. Volkow ND, Wang G, Fowler JS, Logan J, Gerasimov M, Maynard L, et al.
no significant change compared with PLAC for ATM. Our results
(2001): Therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate significantly in-
provide foundational data that might help to explain why acute
crease extracellular dopamine in the human brain. J Neurosci 21:RC121.
doses of MPH have therapeutic value in disorders such as ADHD.
12. Berridge CW, Devilbiss DM, Andrzejewski ME, Arnsten AF, Kelley AE,
Schmeichel B, et al. (2006): Methylphenidate preferentially increases
This work was supported by grants from the Australian Research
catecholamine neurotransmission within the prefrontal cortex at low
Council (DP0770337) to MAB and from the Australian National Health
doses that enhance cognitive function. Biol Psychiatry 60:1111–1120.
13. Bymaster FP, Katner JS, Nelson DL, Hemrick-Luecke SK, Threlkeld PG,
and Medical Research Council to MAB, RLH, PJN, and JBM. We would
Heiligenstein JH, et al. (2002): Atomoxetine increases extracellular levels
like to thank Dr. Chris Chambers for comments on the manuscript. We
of norepinephrine and dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rat: A potential
would also like to thank the Wesley Hospital Pharmacy for dispensing
mechanism for efficacy in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neu-the drugs associated with this project.ropsychopharmacology 27:699 –711. Both LSN and MAB have received reimbursement from Lilly Phar-
14. Arnsten AF, Dudley AG (2005): Methylphenidate improves prefrontal
maceuticals for conference travel expenses and for speaking at confer-
cortical cognitive function through alpha2 adrenoceptor and dopa-
ences. The authors MAB and B-NK have received speaker’s fees from
mine D1 receptor actions: Relevance to therapeutic effects in attention
Jannsen Cilag. The author LSN has received speaker’s fees from Bristol-
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behav Brain Funct 1:2. Myers Squibb, Boehringer, and Janssesn Cilag. The authors RH, JW,
15. Aron AR, Poldrack RA (2006): Cortical and subcortical contributions to
TDRC, KG, AJD, PJN, and JBM report no biomedical financial interests or
stop signal response inhibition: Role of the subthalamic nucleus. J Neu-rosci 26:2424 –2433.
16. Chambers CD, Garavan H, Bellgrove MA (2009): Insights into the neural
Supplementary material cited in this article is available online.
basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33:631– 646.
17. Vaidya CJ, Bunge SA, Dudukovic NM, Zalecki CA, Elliott GR, Gabrieli JD
1. Bellgrove MA, Chambers CD, Vance A, Hall N, Karamitsios M, Bradshaw
(2005): Altered neural substrates of cognitive control in childhood
JL (2006): Lateralized deficit of response inhibition in early-onset schizo-
ADHD: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Am J
phrenia. Psychol Med 36:495–505.
2. Menzies L, Achard S, Chamberlain SR, Fineberg N, Chen CH, del Campo
18. Mink JW (1996): The basal ganglia: Focused selection and inhibition of
N, et al. (2007): Neurocognitive endophenotypes of obsessive-compul-sive disorder. Brain 130:3223–3236.
competing motor programs. Prog Neurobiol 50:381– 425.
3. Lijffijt M, Kenemans JL, Verbaten MN, van Engeland H (2005): A meta-
19. Kuntsi J, Wood AC, Rijsdijk F, Johnson KA, Andreou P, Albrecht B, et al.
analytic review of stopping performance in attention-deficit/hyperac-
(2010): Separation of cognitive impairments in attention-deficit/hyper-
tivity disorder: Deficient inhibitory motor control? J Abnorm Psychol
activity disorder into 2 familial factors. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67:1159 –
4. Logan GD, Schachar RJ, Tannock R (1997): Impulsivity and inhibitory
20. Ramoz N, Boni C, Downing AM, Close SL, Peters SL, Prokop AM, et al.
control. Psychol Sci 8:60 – 64.
(2009): A haplotype of the norepinephrine transporter (Net) gene
5. Bellgrove MA, Hester R, Garavan H (2004): The functional neuroanatomi-
Slc6a2 is associated with clinical response to atomoxetine in attention-
cal correlates of response variability: Evidence from a response inhibi-
deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuropsychopharmacology 34:
tion task. Neuropsychologia 42:1910 –1916.
MASSA FALIDA DO BANCO ROYAL DE INVESTIMENTO S.A. CNPJ nº 21.594.726/0001-70 Classses Valores em R$ 1,00 I - CRÉDITOS COM PRIVILÉGIO GERAL 1) Os assim definidos em outras leis civis e comerciais, salvo disposição contrária da Lei n.o 11.101/05 a) Hon 8.513,52 II - CRÉDITOS QUIROGRAFÁRIOS 1) aqueles não previstos nos demais incisos do art. 83 da Lei 11.101/05009 AGÊ
BJM Global Philosophy and ProcessBJM Global FundsInvesting Offshore with BJM Private Client ServicesBJM Private Client Services offer four managed global funds, each Global Equity of these having a distinct risk return profile designed to meet clients differing We follow a multi-managed approach and use Rand Merchant Bank Global Growth International as our administrators and advisors.